Modelo Matemáticas 2º-5º

Flexibilidad Cognitiva

La Flexibilidad Cognitiva, un componente del funcionamiento ejecutivo, es la capacidad de adaptar efectivamente nuestro comportamiento ante demandas cambiantes del entorno y de las tareas. Los estudiantes pueden usar estas habilidades para transitar entre diferentes actividades en el aula o para pensar sobre un concepto o pregunta desde distintas perspectivas. La Flexibilidad Cognitiva se desarrolla a lo largo de la infancia y la adolescencia. Además, se ha demostrado que este pensamiento adaptativo apoya el pensamiento, el aprendizaje y el éxito en matemáticas de los estudiantes.

Ideas Principales

La Flexibilidad Cognitiva (también llamada “alternancia de tareas” o “desplazamiento de la atención) nos permite pensar sobre múltiples conceptos, ya sea al mismo tiempo o mientras se alterna entre ellos. Por ejemplo, en el cambio de tareas, nos permite atender a diferentes aspectos de una tarea dependiendo de una regla dada (por ejemplo, clasificar formas coloridas por color) y luego cambiar a una nueva regla (por ejemplo, clasificar por forma). La investigación ha encontrado que los estudiantes con TDAH a menudo tienen dificultades con el cambio de tareas, posiblemente debido a una inhibición reducida.

Andersson, U. (2008). Working memory as a predictor of written arithmetical skills in children: The importance of central executive functions. The British Journal of Educational Psychology78(2), 181–203.

Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., & Naglieri, J. A. (2011). Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sampleLearning and Individual Differences21(4), 327–336.

Bock, A. M., Gallaway, K. C., & Hund, A. M. (2015). Specifying links between executive functioning and theory of mind during middle childhood: Cognitive flexibility predicts social understanding. Journal of Cognition and Development16(3), 509–521.

Bowie, C., Harvey, P. D. (2006). Administration and interpretation of Trail Making Test. Nature Protocols1(5), 2277–2281.

Bull, R., & Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children’s mathematics ability: Inhibition, switching, and working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology19(3), 273–293.

Cepeda, N. J., Cepeda, M. L., & Kramer, A. F. (2000). Task switching and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology28(3), 213–226.

Dajani, D. R., & Uddin, L. Q. (2015). Demystifying cognitive flexibility: Implications for clinical and developmental neuroscienceTrends in Neurosciences38(9), 571–578.

Duan, X., Wei, S., Wang, G., & Shi, J. (2010). The relationship between executive functions and intelligence on 11- to 12-year- old childrenPsychological Test and Assessment Modeling52(4), 419–431.

Makar, K. (2014). Young children’s explorations of average through informal inferential reasoningEducational Studies in Mathematics86(1), 61–78.

Matthews, J. S. (2018). When am I ever going to use this in the real world? Cognitive flexibility and urban adolescents’ negotiation of the value of mathematicsJournal of Educational Psychology110(5), 726–746.

Oades, R. D., & Christiansen, H. (2008). Cognitive switching processes in young people with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology23(1), 21-32.

Puente, A. E. (1985). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Test Critiques4, 677–682.

Randazzo, A. C., Muehlbach, M. J., Schweitzer, P. K., & Walsh, J. K. (1998). Cognitive function following acute sleep restriction in children ages 10-14Sleep, 21(8), 861-868.

Rauch, W. A., Gold, A., & Schmitt, K. (2012). To what extent are task-switching deficits in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder independent of impaired inhibition? ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders4(4), 179–187.

Sarsour, K., Sheridan, M., Jutte, D., Nuru-jeter, A., Hinshaw, S., & Boyce, W. T. (2011). Family socioeconomic status and child executive functions: The roles of language, home environment, and single parenthoodJournal of the International Neuropsychological Society17, 120–132.

Spann, M. N., Mayes, L. C., Kalmar, J. H., Guiney, J., Womer, F. Y., Pittman, B., … & Blumberg, H. P. (2012). Childhood abuse and neglect and cognitive flexibility in adolescentsChild Neuropsychology18(2), 182-189.

Spiegler, O., & Leyendecker, B. (2017). Balanced cultural identities promote cognitive flexibility among immigrant children. Frontiers in Psychology8, 1579.

Tombaugh, T. N. (2004). Trail Making Test A and B: Normative data stratified by age and education. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology19(2), 203–214.

Van der Niet, A. G., Hartman, E., Smith, J., & Visscher, C. (2014). Modeling relationships between physical fitness, executive functioning, and academic achievement in primary school childrenPsychology of Sport and Exercise, 15(4), 319-325.

Watson, J. M. (2007). The role of cognitive conflict in developing students’ understanding of averageEducational Studies in Mathematics65(1), 21–47.

Yeniad, N., Malda, M., Mesman, J., Ijzendoorn, M. H. Van, & Pieper, S. (2013). Shifting ability predicts math and reading performance in children: A meta-analytical study. Learning and Individual Differences23, 1–9.