Modelo Lenguaje Prejardín-2º

Flexibilidad Cognitiva

La Flexibilidad Cognitiva, un componente del funcionamiento ejecutivo, es la capacidad de pensar en múltiples conceptos, ya sea al mismo tiempo o alternando entre ellos. La Flexibilidad Cognitiva puede, por ejemplo, ayudar a los lectores a hacer inferencias, al incorporar nueva información mientras leen con su conocimiento previo. La Flexibilidad Cognitiva se desarrolla durante la infancia y se ha demostrado que apoya tanto la lectura de palabras simples como la comprensión más compleja de oraciones y pasajes.

Ideas Principales

La Flexibilidad Cognitiva (también llamada alternancia de tareas o desplazamiento de la atención nos permite pensar sobre múltiples conceptos, ya sea al mismo tiempo o mientras se alterna entre ellos. Utilizamos la Flexibilidad Cognitiva para adaptarnos efectivamente a nuestro comportamiento al enfrentar cambios en el entorno y las demandas de las tareas. En la escuela, los estudiantes deben considerar y alternar entre diferentes reglas y estrategias para completar las tareas de aprendizaje. La investigación ha encontrado que los estudiantes con TDAH a menudo tienen dificultades con la alternancia de tareas, posiblemente debido a una inhibición reducida.

La Flexibilidad Cognitiva puede ayudar a los estudiantes a procesar simultáneamente los sonidos y entender el significado de cada palabra al leer, así como permitirles usar de manera flexible diferentes estrategias de lectura, como releer o hojear, según los objetivos de la tarea.

Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., & Naglieri, J. A. (2011). Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learning and Individual Differences21(4), 327–336.

Bialystok, E. (2011). Global-Local and Trail-Making Tasks by monolingual and bilingual children: Beyond InhibitionDevelopmental Psychology46(1), 93–105.

Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergartenChild Development78(2), 647–663.

Bock, A. M., Gallaway, K. C., & Hund, A. M. (2015). Specifying links between executive functioning and theory of mind during middle childhood: Cognitive flexibility predicts social understandingJournal of Cognition and Development16(3), 509–521.

Bowie, C., Harvey, P. D. (2006). Administration and interpretation of Trail Making Test. Nature Protocols1(5), 2277–2281.

Cepeda, N. J., Cepeda, M. L., & Kramer, A. F. (2000). Task switching and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology28(3), 213–226.

Drijbooms, E., Groen, M. A., & Verhoeven, L. (2015). The contribution of executive functions to narrative writing in fourth grade children. Reading and Writing28, 989–1011.

Kieffer, M. J., Vukovic, R. K., & Berry, D. (2013). Roles of attention shifting and inhibitory control in fourth-grade reading comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly48(4), 333–348.

Nouwens, S., Groen, M. A., & Verhoeven, L. (2016). How storage and executive functions contribute to children’s reading comprehension. Learning and Individual Differences47, 96-102.

Poljac, E., Simon, S., Ringlever, L., & Bekkering, H. (2010). Impaired task switching performance in children with dyslexia but not in children with autism. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology63(2), 401–416.

Randazzo, A. C., Muehlbach, M. J., Schweitzer, P. K., & Walsh, J. K. (1998). Cognitive function following acute sleep restriction in children ages 10-14Sleep, 21(8), 861-868.

Sarsour, K., Sheridan, M., Jutte, D., Nuru-Jeter, A., Hinshaw, S., & Boyce, W. T. (2010). Family socioeconomic status and child executive functions: The roles of language, home environment, and single parenthood. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society17, 120–132.

Spann, M. N., Mayes, L. C., & Kalmar, J. H. (2012). Childhood abuse and neglect and cognitive flexibility in adolescents. Child Neuropsychology18(2), 182–189.

Vitiello, V. E., Greenfield, D. B., Munis, P., & George, J. (2011). Cognitive flexibility, approaches to learning, and academic school readiness in head start preschool children. Early Education and Development, 22(3), 338–410.

Randazzo, A. C., Muehlbach, M. J., Schweitzer, P. K., & Walsh, J. K. (1998). Cognitive function following acute sleep restriction in children ages 10-14. Pediatric Sleep21(8), 861–868.

Rauch, W. A., Gold, A., & Schmitt, K. (2012). To what extent are task-switching deficits in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder independent of impaired inhibition? ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders4(4), 179–187.

Sarsour, K., Sheridan, M., Jutte, D., Nuru-jeter, A., Hinshaw, S., & Boyce, W. T. (2010). Family socioeconomic status and child executive functions: The roles of language, home environment, and single parenthood. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society17, 120–132.

Spann, M. N., Mayes, L. C., & Kalmar, J. H. (2012). Childhood abuse and neglect and cognitive flexibility in adolescents. Child Neuropsychology18(2), 182–189.

Van der Niet, A. G., Hartman, E., Smith, J., & Visscher, C. (2014). Modeling relationships between physical fitness, executive functioning, and academic achievement in primary school children. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 15(4), 319-325.

Vitiello, V. E., Greenfield, D. B., Munis, P., & George, J. (2011). Cognitive flexibility, approaches to learning, and academic school readiness in head start preschool children. Early Education and Development22(3), 338–410.

Yeniad, N., Malda, M., Mesman, J., Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., & Pieper, S. (2013). Shifting ability predicts math and reading performance in children: A meta-analytical study. Learning and Individual Differences23(1), 1–9.